THE CRITICAL PERIOD HYPOTHESIS
The notion that a biologically determined period exists during which language
acquisition must occur, if it is to occur at all, is known as the critical period
hypothesis. Nature provides many examples of biologically determined
deadlines, the best known of which is probably the critical period for imprinting
in birds. Some species of birds walk as soon as they hatch. Chicks
and ducklings, for example, will follow the first moving thing they see, and
then they will follow it forever. Normally, the first thing a baby sees when it
hatches is its mother, with the result that chicks and ducklings then follow
their mothers everywhere. When this occurs, the chicks and ducklings are said
to be imprinted on their mother. Imprinting cannot happen any time; it must
occur within a few hours after hatching. (This requirement is not always absolute,
and the term sensitive period is sometimes substituted for critical period
[Lieberman, 1993].)
There are well-documented human examples of critical periods as well.
For example, some cells in the brain respond to input from both eyes in the
normal adult, but if these cells fail to receive input from two eyes during the
first year or two of life, they lose this capacity. Thus, the features common to
all examples of critical or sensitive periods are these: some environmental input
is necessary for normal development, but biology determines when the
organism is responsive to that input. That period of responsivity is the critical
period. If one were to design a study to test the critical period hypothesis, one
would deprive children of exposure to language during the normal period of
language development, provide the exposure later, and examine the languag~
development that occurs. Of course, such an unethical experiment could
never be done deliberately, but history has provided a few such cases.
..Wild" children
Victor of Aveyron was such a case in which first exposure to language, so far
as we know, came late. As mentioned in Chapter 1, Victor never acquired norrnallanguage.
There are a few other cases of such wild children who also did
not learn language under the circumstance of early deprivation and late first
exposure, but it is difficult to learn much from such cases because they are so
poorly,documented. Furthermore, when such children fail to acquire language,
we cannot be sure whether the failure was due to the late start or to
some impairment the child might have had previously. Reviewing the evidence
in 1967, Lenneberg came to the conclusion that "the only safe conclusions
to be drawn from the multitude of reports is life in dark closets, wolves'
dens, forests, or sadistic parents' backyards is not conducive to good health
and normal development" (p. 142).
There is one success story among such children. In the 1930s, a 6-year-old
child named Isabelle was discovered living hidden away in a dark room with
only her deaf-mute mother for contact. After her discovery, Isabelle was trained
intensively to speak, and she did learn to talk. Isabelle's success makes it clear
that she was cognitively normal, but her deprivation was also less extreme than
that of the other cases of wild children. Furthermore, we do not have the sort
of psycholinguistic details about Isabelle we would like. Although at age 8 she
was described as having "a normal IQ" and "not easily distinguished from ordinary
children of her age" (Brown, 1958a,p. 192), no one administered the tests
of linguistic competence that would allow detailed comparisons of her language
competence with that of children with normal experience.
There is one modern case of a wild child who was discovered after linguistics
and neurology were sufficiently advanced to allow us to ask questions
that were not asked of the earlier cases.
The case of Genie
In 1970 a woman who is known to most of the world only as "Genie's mother"
was lookiIig for the office of services for the blind in downtown Los Angeles.
She was nearly blind, was seekiIig help for herself, and had only recently
managed to escape virtual captivity by her mentally ill husband. By mistake,
she entered the general social services office. She brought with her a 13-yearold
daughter, Genie. The eligibility worker at the social services office noticed
the small, frail-looking child with a strange gait and posture and called her supervisor,
who, after questioning Genie's mother, called the police. The police
took Genie into custody and admitted her into the hospital for severe malnutrition
(Curtiss, 1977; Rymer, 1993).
The story of Genie's background that was eventually revealed was horrific.
From the time Genie was 20 months old until her mother's escape when
she was 13, Genie spent her time alone, strapped to a potty chair in a small
bedroom. She was fed hurriedly, with minimal interaction and no talk. If
Genie made any noise, her father would beat her with a large piece of wood
he kept in the room for that purpose. Like the Wild Boy of Aveyron before
her, Genie had no language when she was discovered. Also like Victor of
Aveyron, Genie was immensely interesting to the scientific community. The
story of Genie's life and treatment both before and after her discovery have
been described by Curtiss (1977) and by Rymer (1993). We shall confine ourselves
here to the investigation of Genie's language development, described
by Susan Curtiss in her dissertation and subsequent papers (Curtiss, 1977,
1985, 1988, 1989).
Genie did not talk at all when she was first discovered. Four years later,
she scored in the range of a normal 5-year-old on standardized vocabulary
tests. She combined words into complex utterances, and she could express
meanings. However, her language was far from normal. As the examples of
Genie's speech in Box 2.4 show, her vocabulary and semantic skills far ex
Mamawash hair in sink.
At school scratch face.
I want Curtiss play piano.
Like go ride yellow school bus.
Father take piece wood. Hit. Cry.
Source' Curtiss, 1977.
ceeded her syntactic skills. Her grammar was deficient in both production and
comprehension. In production, her utterances were telegraphic, lacking most
grammatical morphemes. In comprehension tests, she failed to understand
passive constructions and distinctions marked by tense, and she had other difficulties
as well.
Another fact about Genie's language might be related to her grammatical
limitations. Dichotic listening tests showed that language was a right-hemisphere
activity for Genie. In fact, the nature of her grammatical limitations has been
compared to the grammatical deficiencies of patients who have recovered language
after surgical removal of the left hemisphere. One possible explanation
of this phenomenon is that Genie was exposed to language too late for the
normal process of acquisition of language as a left-hemisphere function. She
acquired language with the right hemisphere, and-as we have seen in the
aphasia data-the right hemisphere is not as good at language as the left.
Genie's conversational competence was also extremely limited, and she often
ignored the speech addressed to her. As Curtiss (1977) described it:
Verbal interaction with Genie consists mainly of someone's asking
Genie a question repeatedly until Genie answers, or of Genie's
making a comment and someone else's responding to it in some way.
...Except for those instances where Genie exerts control over the
topic through repetition, verbal interaction with Genie is almost
always controlled and/or "normalized" by the person talking to
Genie, not by Genie. (p. 233)
Curtiss attributes this conversational incompetence to Genie's lack of
early socializing experience.
The study of Genie is certainly more informative than earlier reports on
isolated children. Evidence that language was a right-hemisphere function for
Genie suggests that by age 13, a left hemisphere that has never been used for
language has lost that capacity. However, interpretation of Genie's outcome is
still hampered by the fact that we do not know with certainty that Genie was
a normal child except for her experiences. Once, when Genie was seen by a
doctor as an infant, she was diagnosed as mentally retarded. But there was
never any follow-up to see whether that pediatrician's impression was correct,
and even before Genie was totally isolated, she had something less than an
ideal environment. Susan Curtiss, who worked most closely with Genie, vehemendy
disagrees with the possibility that Genie could be retarded (Rymer,
1993), but we simply do not know for sure.
Late acQuisition of American Sign Language
A better test of the critical period hypothesis is provided by individuals who
have normal early experience except for being deprived of exposure to language.
This is the circumstance of many children born deaf to hearing parents.
These children have no language input at home because they cannot
hear the language their family speaks, and their parents do not know sign language
(and historically have been discouraged from learning it to communicate
with their children, although this has changed in recent years). Many of
these children are eventually exposed to sign language when they meet other
deaf children, some of whom have deaf parents and have been exposed to
sign language from infancy. Comparing the sign language acquisition of children
who learned sign from infancy to that of children who were first exposed
to it later in childhood or in adulthood provides a very nice test of the critical
period hypothesis. If the young brain is better at language acquisition, deaf individuals
who began to acquire sign as older children should be less proficient
than those who acquired it in infancy.
Newport (1990) studied the sign language proficiency of deaf adults who
ranged in age from 35 to 70, who used American Sign Language (ASL) in their
everyday communication, and who had done so for more than 30 years. Some
of these adults had acquired ASL as infants from their deaf parents. Some had
first been exposed to ASL when they entered a school for the deaf between
the age of 4 and 6; some had first been exposed only after the age of 12, when
they entered the school as teenagers, or later, when they made friends with
or married someone from that school. Newport administered a battery of comprehension
and production tests to assess how well these deaf adult.5 had
mastered the grammar of ASL. She found that adults who were first exposed
to ASL after early childhood-even after 30 years of using the language every
day-<iid not perform as well as those who had been exposed as infants. Similarly,
Mayberry and Eichen (1991) found that early learners of sign had an advantage
over late learners in recalling and reproducing ASL sentences that
were presented to them. This evidence suggests there is some benefit to being
a young language learner.
,;1
The evidence from second language acQuisition
A far mor~ frequently occurring test of the effect of age on language acquisition
occurs in the realm of second language acquisition, and to the casual observer,
the results are obvious. Children learn a new language readily, and
soon after moving to a new lan~age community they are indistinguishable
from their native-born peers. Adults, in contrast, master a new language only
with difficulty and never quite sound like native speakers. The gist of these
everyday observations has been frequently supported by experimental tests:
the younger one is when exposed to a second language, the better one's ultimate
proficiency in that language. That fact does not necessarily support the
critical period hypothesis, however. The hypothesis that biology sets a cutoff
age after which language cannot be learned to native-like competence predicts
an abrupt age-related change in the success of language learning that
cannot be explained by other factors. As we shall see, the details of the findings
regarding age and second language learning do not consistently support
that strong hypothesis.
Age effects on second language acquisition. Among adults who have
emigrated to the United States from a non-English speaking country, their age
of arrival in the United States predicts the degree to which they will have a
foreign accent and their performance on grammatical judgment tasks. In both
cases, the younger the immigrants were when they were first exposed to English,
the more like native speakers they sound and perform.
In a study by Oyarna (1976), the English speech of60 Italian immigrants
was tape-recorded, and two judges scored those records on a five-point scale
ranging from no foreign accent to heavy foreign accent. Oyama then analyzed
the influence of two variables: (1) the age of the immigrants at arrival in the
United States and (2) the number of years living in the United States. Oyama
found a strong effect of age at arrival, with young arrivals showing less
accent than older ones. The number of years had no effect. Oyama (1978)
similarly found strong age-of-arrival effects on second language users' ability
to repeat English sentences presented to them under noisy conditions (i.e.,
static on the tape).
Other studies have similarly found that age of arrival affects the ability to
speak 'a second language without an accent and that native-like performance
depends on exposure beginning in early childhood (Flege, 1987; Flege &
Fletcher, 1992). Sometimes the benefit of youth to acquiring unaccented speech
in a second language is explained as the effect of age on acquiring a motor
skill. Speech production involves moving the lips, tongue, and mouth in ways
particular to each language, and that may be what is difficult for a late learner.
However, there is more to knowing the sound system of a language than just
the motor skill, and Oyama's (1978) finding of effects of age of arrival on com
prehensionsuggests that not just the mouth, but also the brairi, is involved iri
explaining age effects on the mastery of second language phonology .
A similar effect has been observed for measures of grammatical competence.
Johnson and Newport (1989) presented grammatical and ungrammatical
English sentences to Chinese and Korean natives who were living in the
United States and who had learned English as a second language. As a group,
they did less well on identifying the ungrammatical utterances than a comparison
group of native English speakers. The interesting results with respect
to the critical period hypothesis come from an analysis of subgroups of the
second language speakers. Those who were between 3 and 7 years old when
they arrived in the United States were not different from the native speakers
of English. Those who were between 8 and 15 at airival performed less well
on the test than native speakers, but the younger they were at airival, the
more nearly they approx.imated native competence. Those who were 17 or
older performed least well and did no better than those who were 30. As was
the case for accent, differences were observed between those exposed as
young children, under the age of 7, and those exposed as older, but still prepubescent,
children. Additional evidence of age-of .airival effects comes from
Coppieters (1987), who found that native speakers outperformed adult second
language learners on tests of grammatical competence-even when the second
language learners worked as authors and professors, writing in their second
language.
Continuity or discontinuity in age-of-arrival effects. Although it is clear
that the age at which one starts to learn a language is related to one's ultimate
level of proficiency in that language, it is not clear that the cause of this relation
is a change in the brain's specific capacity for language acquisition. The
hypothesis that there is a biologically defined window during which language
acquisition must occur predicts a discontinuity in the relation of age to ultimate
proficiency. Proficiency should be good for those exposed during the
critical period and less good for those exposed outside of the critical period.
Some evidence suggests that, to the contrary, the function that relates age of
first exposure to ultimate proficiency is a smooth one.
One study that found a continuous function mapping age of arrival to language
competence used census data collected from 2 million immigrants, all of
whom had been living in the United States for at least 10 years (Hakuta, Bialstok,
& Wiley, 2003). The res~ndents reported how proficient they considered
themselves to be in English, their age when they immigrated to the United
States, and the level of education they had attained. The findings are presented
in Figure 2.6 for native speakers of Chinese and Spanish. There is clear evidence
that age of immigration makes a difference, but there is no clear
evidence of a discontinuity .The observed effect of education level also argues
against a biological account of proficiency differences. Although one potential
criticism of these data is that the self-report measure was not very sensitive, it
Native Chinese speakers
Native Spanish speakers>-4
u
~, , ~ .QuO J" ' .:= 3
" ' 0
." ',::: College t..
~'. ---,
"'~ --:::-:::::~HSgra d .J":': 2
."..'::: --
HS ~: <8 yrs ed bQI)
<5 yrs ~d ~
>-.4
u
~0;
.0
~ 3
8
p.
.!:
.~ 2
~
1
10 20 40 60 0
Age of immigration
Figure 2.6 The relation between age of immigration and English proficiency
for native Chinese and Spanish speakers who emigrated to the United States
Source. From "Critical Evidence. A Test of the Critical-Period Hypothesis for Second-Language
Acquisition." Psychological Science, 2003, 14, 31-38. Copyright 2003 American Psychological
Association.
20 40 60
Age of immigration
was sufficiently sensitive to reveal the age of immigration effect. More seriously,
it has been suggested that Hakuta et al.'s (2003) data analytic techniques
might have obscured evidence of a discontinuity in the age effect (Stevens,
2004). On the other hand, other studies have also found that even after puberty
, younger is better with respect to second language acquisition and that
some adults (approximately 5 to 25 percent, depending on the study) can
achieve native-like proficiency in a second language. These other findings similarly
suggest that a simple "puberty closes the window of opportunity" account
of age effects on second language acquisition does not fully explain the data.
Nonbiological influences on second language acQUisition
Another argument against the critical period hypothesis is based on evidence
that factors other than biological age contribute to the age-related decline in second
language learning achievement. Typically, the input conditions for children
and adults are different. Children attend school in the new language, whereas
adults must do work that their limited language skills allow, thus limiting their
exposure to the new language. Jia and Aaronson studied Chinese immigrants to
the United States who were between 5 and 16 years old for their first year in
their new country (Jia & Aaronson, 2003). They found that the children under
age 2 were exposed to a richer English-language environment than the older
children. These very young children watched more English-language television,
were exposed to more English-language books, and had more English-speaking
friends. The older children had richer Chinese language environments. Although
all the children reported they were more comfortable using Chinese when they
first arrived, by the end of the first year the younger children, but not the older,
reported being more comfortable using English. The younger children also
scored higher than the older children on tests of English profidency .If young
children, but not older children or adults, switch their dominant language when
inimigrating, then the studies that look for effects of age on language-learning
ability by comparing younger and older arrivals have a problem.
To the extent that comparison of the language proficiency of young to adult
learners of a second language is a comparison of language proficiency in speakers
for whom that language is dominant to speakers for whom that language is
not, the validity of that research is compromised. It may be that switching the
dominant language is what causes the differences, not age per se. To investigate
this dominant language switch hypothesis, Jia and Aaronson (2003) gave
grammaticality judgment tasks in English and Chinese to native Chinese who
had come to the United States between the ages of 1 and 38. Like Johnson and
Newport, they found that younger arrivals performed better on the test of
English ~. They also found that the younger arrivals performed worse on
the test of Chinese grammar. In fact, scores on the two tests were negatively
related-.,-the better a subject did on the English test, the worse he or she did on
the Chinese test. This finding supports the hypothesis that better second language
acquisition by younger children occurs at least in part because they switch
to the second language as their dominant language.
Further support for the contention that the language-learning advantage
of young children is not purely a reflection of a greater language-learning ability
comes from a study of the acquisition of Dutch as a second language by
English speakers who moved to the Netherlands (Snow & Hoefnagle-Hohle,
1978). The subjects ranged in age from 3 years to adult. Snow and Hoefnagel-
Hohle tested their participants' mastery of Dutch using a variety of measures
of pronunciation, vocabulary, grammar, and text comprehension, first 6
months after their arrival and then two more times at 4- to 5-month intervals.
Contrary to the prediction of the critical period hypothesis, they found that the
youngest children scored the lowest on every test and that the 12- to 15-yearolds
showed the most rapid acquisition.
Also contrary to a biologically based account of the advantage of youth
in second language acquisition, there is evidence that social psychological
variables playa role in second language acquisition. Johnson and Newport
found two such variables that affected the performance in English of their Korean
and Chinese participants: self -consciousness and American identification.
T'ne participaflts who were less self -conscious about making errors and those
who identified themselves as Ainerican showed greater mastery of English.
Both the characteristics of not being self -conscious and of identifying with the
new country are more likely to be true of children than adults, and thus these
noribiological factors may also contribute to the observed age difference in
second language acquisition.
The data on second language acquisition suggest that if you give younger
children and older children the same experience with a second language, the
b I
older children will learn more rapidly. However, in most immigrant situations,
it is the younger children who actually learn more and ultimately achieve
higher level~ of proficiency. Some of the disadvantage that older learners have
may be the result of general cognitive declines that come with age, including
declines in memory , attention, and speed of processing. Some of the disadvantage
of older learners is likely due to other, noncognitive factors including
the conditions of input, their initial high level of proficiency in their native language,
and their reluctance to switch cultural identities and language.
In sum, although it is widely believed that language acquisition is "a
mysterious skill that seems to shut off automatically around the age of 12"
(Osborne, 2003, p. 40), this view is simply not supported by the evidence. The
evidence from Genie, from studies of sign language acquisition, and from recovery
from aphasia show that children have an advantage over adults. However,
that general conclusion leaves open many questions about the basis and
the time of any critical period. Lenneberg (1967), who originally hypothesized
the existence of a biologically based critical period for language acquisition,
proposed puberty as the deadline. According to Bates (1993), however, the capacity
for recovery from aphasia begins to decline after age 5. Newport's 1990
data on the acquisition of American Sign Language show a decline in ultimate
achievement between those first exposed at 6 and those first exposed at 12,
and another decline from 12 to adulthood. None of these sources of data supports
the notion of puberty as a deadline or of an abrupt deadline at any age.
The latgest source of data on the critical period hypothesis, which is also
the source of most people's belief in it, is the observation of age-related differences
in second language acquisition. What these data appear to show,
however, is a continuous decline in ultimate language proficiency related to
the age of first exposure-not an abrupt shutting-off of ability. Furthermore,
some of the decline in language achievement may have nothing to do with
language-learning ability. Rather, other sources of influence including conditions
of exposure and willingness to identify with the new language group
give children an advantage over adults. To the extent that the decline does reflect
a decline in ability, it is not necessarily a language-specific ability. All
sorts of general cognitive processes decline with age (Hakuta et al. , 2003). Additionally,
it has been proposed that one way in which children's cognitive
abilities are limited relative to adults' may provide a language-learning advantage'.
Newport (1991) has argued that it is easier to figure out the structure
of language if you analyze small chunks than if you analyze longer stretches
of speech. Small chunks are all that children can extract from input and store
in memory. Adults, in contrast, extract and store larger chunks, thereby giving
themselves a more-difficult analytical task. Newport refers to this argument as
the "less is more" hypothesis. This hypothesis has also received support
from a computer simulation of language acquisition done within the connectionist
approach, which found that the computer was more successful if fed
~(J
shorter sequences as input (Elman, 1993, 2001), although that finding has
been disputed (Rohde & Plaut, 1999).
One possible reconciliation of the data with respect to the critical or, more
likely, sensitive period hypothesis is that there is a biological preparedness for
language acquisition that is maximal in early childhood. Language acquisition
also depends on other factors, including general cognitive processes and motivation.
The benefit of these other factors typically decline more gradually than
the biological advantage and are more variable after puberty. Thus, early childhood
is the easiest time to acquire language, but it is doable after early childhood
if other factors are maximally supportive.
THE GENETIC BASIS OF LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
The genetic basis of language universals
All normal children in anything remotely like a normal environment learn to
talk. Furthermore, the course of Janguage development is, in broad outline,
constant across varying environments. The argument has been made that this
invariance and robustness of language development reflects a genetic plan at
work. Language development is to a large degree a maturational process according
to this view-its course and timing determined by the unfolding of a
genetic blueprint CGilger, 1996; Gleitman, 1981).
On the other hand, it has been argued that the universal acquisition of
language is the result of universal features of human environments. Although
the circumstances under which children acquire language vary
widely, all environments appear to provide two forms of support for the
process of language acquisition. All environments show children that language
is used to communicate with other people, and all environments provide
children with samples of speech in a manner that allows children to figure
out the relations between the sound and meaning CCrago, Allen, &
Hough-Eyamie, 1997; Lieven, 1994). This is not to say that language acquisition
does not also depend on biological properties of humans. Rather, the
biologically provided language-acquisition device depends on finding a certain
sort of environment-just as the biologically provided program for
physical growth depends on nutrition.
Thehetitabillty of Individual djfferentes
Just as the fact that all humans acquire language has been attributed to our
shared genetic blueprint, the individual differences among children in their rate
of language development have been attributed, to some degree, to genetic differences.
The field that studies the genetic basis of individual differences in behavioral
characteristics is behavior genetics, and the tools of behavior genetics
Post a Comment